New Discovery Neuroscience and Psychology Published: December 18, 2020

How Do Teens and Adults Learn About Other People?

Abstract

Imagine that you meet someone new. You may wonder what they like, for example how much do they like baseball? You then get their feedback, which helps you to predict how much they like something similar, like basketball. We tested how teens and adults decide what others like and dislike and how they learn about others through feedback. This learning process can be described with mathematical models that calculate prediction errors—the difference between how much you think someone likes baseball and their actual preference for it. Teens and adults differed in how quickly they learned about others using this measure. Teens also tended to use a different brain region than adults when learning about the preferences of other people. This study helps us to understand how social learning develops over teenage years.

How Do You Learn About Other People?

You probably often meet new people who are around your age—new students at your school, new members of your sports or music club, new neighbors, and so on. Think back on the last time that you met a new person. At the beginning, you did not know much about them. But, very quickly, you probably got an idea about what the new person likes. How much do they like candy, baseball, or sneakers? Knowing how much a person likes certain objects and activities may help you to predict their preferences for other objects and activities. For example, if they like apple pie, you may think that they also like carrot cake, even though they never told you that they like it. Your predictions about your new peers may affect how you interact with them. If you know that they like baseball, for instance, you might think that they also like basketball and invite them to a basketball game. Your knowledge about other people shapes your relationships with them. For example, a friend may be irritated if you give her a football as a birthday present even though she told you in the past that she does not like rugby and other ball sports. Figure 1 shows an example of how a “blue person” thinks about the preferences of a new “green person” and how this new person gives them feedback.

Figure 1 - The blue persons meets the green person and wonders about how much the green person likes different items.
  • Figure 1 - The blue persons meets the green person and wonders about how much the green person likes different items.
  • The green person gives feedback for some items.

Why Might Learning Differ Between Teens and Adults?

Learning about others is an important type of social learning. Social learning can be helpful, but it can also be tricky. You need to think carefully about the other person’s preferences for many different objects and activities. Ideally, over time, you can update your predictions about what a person likes as you get feedback from them. As you grow older, you may become more used to meeting new people because it happens more often. Therefore, teens and adults may differ in how they learn about their peers. In our study, we compared social learning in teens and adults. We looked at brain activity during social interactions when people meet each other [1, 2]. We used a technique called functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) [3] to test which brain regions contribute to social learning. We expected that the brain regions involved in social learning would differ between teens and adults, because the brain continues to develop during adolescence [4].

What Did the Learning Task in Our Study Look Like?

We invited 24 teens (between 10 and 17 years of age) and 21 adults (between 23 and 36 years of age) to take part in a study of brain activity using fMRI (Figure 2A) [3]. We designed an experimental task that is similar to getting to know the preferences of a new person in real life. Participants in our study did not directly meet a new person. Instead, they saw a short description of the other person, for example of a student called Mathew (Figure 2B). Participants then saw a picture of an object or activity on the screen, such as an apple. Participants were asked to predict how much Mathew liked apples on a 10-point scale from 1 (not at all) to 10 (very much). After giving their prediction, participants got feedback and saw which rating on the scale Mathew had given for apples. This was repeated for many different objects from three main categories: (1) food items such as fruits, vegetables, sweets, fast foods, and healthy foods; (2) fashion items, such as clothes, shoes, bags, and cosmetics; and (3) activity items, such as toys, gadgets, musical instruments, sports equipment and painting materials. In three sessions, participants learned about three different people like Mathew. Teens learned about other teens and adults learned about other adults. At the end of the experiment, participants rated how much they themselves liked the objects and activities, which allowed us to test if they used their own preferences to predict the preferences of Matthew and the other people (Figure 2C). Participants were also asked to write down their impressions of Mathew and the other people they learned about.

Figure 2 - Our experimental setup.
  • Figure 2 - Our experimental setup.
  • (A) The participant completed our social learning task while in an fMRI scanner. (B) While in the scanner, participants saw short descriptions of new people and learned about their preferences for items using a rating task. (C) After the fMRI scan, participants filled out a survey to indicate their own preferences for the same items as presented in the rating task.

How Can Learning About Others Be Measured?

We can use a calculation to show how good participants were at predicting the preferences of the other people. To do this, we can simply take the difference between participants’ predictions and the feedback they got. This difference is called the prediction error. In the example shown in Figure 2B, the participant predicted that Mathew does not like apples very much and would rate them a “4.” But the participant received the feedback that Mathew liked apples quite a lot. Mathew chose the rating “8.” The prediction error here is 4, because 8 – 4 = 4. The smaller the prediction error, the better participants are at predicting. We found that prediction errors got smaller over time as participants got more feedback about the person. This learning happened for both teens and adults. But overall, teens had slightly higher prediction errors, which might indicate that teens have less experience with social learning than adults. The written descriptions of the other people that teens and adults gave at the end of the experiment were very detailed, which highlights that both age groups were able to learn about the preferences of their peers in our task.

Many other studies have investigated how humans learn and update their predictions using simple tasks that do not involve learning about other people. These studies have developed detailed mathematical descriptions of the learning process [5]. In our study, we tested whether this type of mathematical models can be extended to our work on social learning about the preferences of other people. We investigated how well different mathematical models captured how participants rated the preferences of others. A good mathematical model should be relatively simple and predict participants’ ratings well.

Which Mathematical Models Describe Learning About Others?

Our three main models made different assumptions about how people rate the preferences of others. The first model was that participants do not learn at all. When you meet a new person, you could just think that this person likes exactly what you like. This may be smart in the beginning but over time you should learn how the other person differs from you.

Our second main model is a commonly used learning model, called the reinforcement learning model. This model has been used often to describe learning in both animals and humans [5]. Based on this model, participants learn about their surroundings through feedback. Let us say that participants are presented with two pictures, one is more and one less likely to give participants a reward. If participants choose one of the two pictures and then get a reward, they learn that it is better to pick that picture in the future. The way they learn is by updating their prediction error: the difference between their prediction that the picture gives a reward and the outcome. This prediction error will influence which picture participants choose in the future.

Our third main model combined the two models described above. This combination model was best at describing participants’ ratings, both for teens and for adults. Teens were slower than adults at updating their predictions. Slower updating is not necessarily bad. Updating too quickly has the danger that recent feedback is weighted too much. As an example, if you think your friend likes all chocolate bars and you then learn that she dislikes chocolate bars with peanuts, you should not use her response for peanut chocolate bars too quickly to update your prediction of how much she likes chocolate bars in general. Overall, the prediction updating speed was related to how old the teens were, which further suggests that learning changes with age.

Which Brain Regions Help Us Learn About Others?

We linked the mathematical models to participants’ fMRI data [3]. By doing this, we could see which brain regions were related to participants’ predictions of the other persons’ preferences and to predictions errors. A brain region is related to the processing of these numbers if its activity scales with these numbers. That is, such a region shows higher activity during time points with higher prediction errors and lower activity during time points with smaller prediction errors.

We expected activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), a region in the middle front part of the brain behind the forehead, to be involved in thinking about the preferences of others. This brain region has been linked to complex learning processes and to social thought processes, such as thinking about oneself and about other people [1, 2]. The MPFC also undergoes a lot of developmental change during adolescence [4], which is not the case for many other brain regions that develop earlier. Indeed, we found that activity in the MPFC reflected the predictions of adults and less so of teens (Figure 3). In contrast, another brain region, called the fusiform gyrus, which is in the back of the brain, was related to prediction errors in teens but not in adults. This suggests a developmental shift in the brain regions that are involved in social learning.

Figure 3 - Teens and adults seem to use different brain regions to learn about the preferences of others.
  • Figure 3 - Teens and adults seem to use different brain regions to learn about the preferences of others.
  • (A) In adults, activity in the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) relates to their ratings of what another kid might like. (B) Teens more often use a brain area called the fusiform gyrus, a region in the back of the brain. The numbers on the y-axis are in arbitrary units (a.u.), which means that the exact magnitudes cannot be interpreted directly.

Future Questions

There are many things to learn about other people. For example, what would they do in various situations? Would they share with you or keep everything for themselves? It would be interesting to know if learning about such activities is similar to learning about preferences1. Additionally, some people have difficulties understanding what others think or feel. For example, autistic teens might show differences in social learning. If so, it would be interesting to know if different brain regions are involved in the social learning of kids with autism and if training can help them.

Our study shows how mathematical models and brain activity can describe differences in how teens and adults learn about other people. This is important for understanding how teens develop.

Glossary

Social Learning: A term that summarizes the process of how people learn about, from, or for other people. For example, learning about other people’s preferences is a type of social learning.

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): A way to measure brain activity during a task. Participants lie in a machine that produces pictures of brain activity.

Prediction Errors: The differences between predictions and outcomes or feedback. For example, a prediction error can be the difference between your prediction that somebody likes chocolate a lot and the outcome or feedback that this person does not like chocolate at all.

Mathematical Models: Mathematical descriptions of what people do in real life and in experimental studies.

Reinforcement Learning: A specific and very powerful type of mathematical model that describes learning based on prediction errors.

Medial Prefrontal Cortex (MPFC): A region in the middle front part of the brain behind the forehead. This large part of the brain is involved in many processes, such as learning, making decisions, and thinking about others.

Fusiform Gyrus: A region in the a region in the back of the brain, which has been linked to learning and to perceiving and recognizing different types of stimuli, such as different faces.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Athina Tzovara and Julia Kam for organizing the live review for kids at OHBM.

Footnotes

1. If you want to read more about the neural bases of social learning about and from other people, check out this paper [6].


Original Source Article

Rosenblau, G., Korn, C. W., and Pelphrey, K. A. 2018. A computational account of optimizing social predictions reveals that adolescents are conservative learners in social contexts. J. Neurosci. 38:974–88. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1044-17.2017


References

[1] Ruff, C. C., and Fehr, E. 2014. The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15:549–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn3776

[2] Yang, D. Y., Rosenblau, G., Keifer, C., and Pelphrey, K. A. 2015. An integrative neural model of social perception, action observation, and theory of mind. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 51:263–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.01.020

[3] Hoyos, P. M., Kim, N. Y., and Kastner, S. 2019. How is magnetic resonance imaging used to learn about the brain? Front. Young Minds 7:86. doi: 10.3389/frym.2019.00086

[4] Mills, K., Goddings, A., and Blakemore, S. 2014. Drama in the teenage brain. Front. Young Minds 2:16. doi: 10.3389/frym.2014.00016

[5] Niv, Y., and Langdon, A. 2016. Reinforcement learning with Marr. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 11:67–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.005

[6] Koele, I., Westhoff, B., and van de Groep, I. 2020. Social learning and the brain: how do we learn from and about other people? Front. Young Minds 8:95. doi: 10.3389/frym.2020.00095