Core Concept Neuroscience and Psychology Published: May 20, 2022

How Do Our Brains Support Our Friendships?

Abstract

Have you ever wondered how your friends impact how you see the world? Or how you are able to keep track of the many different people in your life? To study these questions, scientists have begun to look at people’s social networks and their brains at the same time. In this article, we introduce this area of study and discuss how scientists use ideas from both neuroscience and mathematics to examine these questions. We also highlight some recent discoveries that reveal both how our brains support our ability to socialize with others and how our relationships with other people are related to how we use our brains.

Your Complex Social Life

Think about the people you know. Who comes to mind? You might think first about your closest friends. You might also think about the people who are the most popular in your grade, such as the kids with the most friends or those who are well-known by everyone else in school. How are you able to keep track of all of these different people and their relationships with each other? The human brain has mechanisms to support the ability to keep track not only of all of the different people in our lives, but also of the complicated relationships between those people. Scientists have begun to look at how the ways that we use our brains are related to features of our social worlds, like the number of friends that we have and whether we play an influential role in our social groups.

Studying Social Networks

How do scientists study the relationships between people’s social worlds and the ways that they use their brains? For many years, neuroscientists (that is, scientists who study the brain) have studied people’s brains without considering the social environment in which people live. Meanwhile, other scientists—including mathematicians, statisticians, and sociologists—have developed many tools to study networks. There are many different types of networks. Some examples are networks of roads between locations in a city, flights between airports, power lines between houses, and the World Wide Web. However, people are particularly attuned to one specific type of network: our social networks. Most typically, a social network is a collection of people who know each other and the relationships between those people [1, 2]. Recently, scientists have begun to combine the study of brains with the study of social networks to help us understand how our brains support our ability to socialize and connect with other people.

Scientists can study a social network in many ways. Suppose that a researcher seeks to characterize the social network of students at your school based on friendships. To do this, the researcher can study which students are friends with each other. In network-science terms, the students are called nodes and each friendship between two students is called an edge (see Figure 1).

Figure 1 - A social network of students.
  • Figure 1 - A social network of students.
  • In network-science terms, each student is a node and each friendship between two students is an edge. In this figure, we represent edges by drawing lines between students. We calculate the distance between two students by counting the number of edges that are needed to connect them. For example, the distance between Felicity and Marco is 2. (Image credit: Adobe Stock File #221784609)

Perhaps we seek to identify the most influential student in your school. Depending on the type of influence that interests us, we can do this in various ways. If we are interested in identifying the most popular student, we can calculate what network scientists call degree. We calculate the degree of each student in your school’s social network by counting the number of friends of each student in the school. For instance, maybe the most popular student in the school has 54 friends; that student has a degree of 54. If we are instead interested in identifying the students who are good at connecting different groups of friends, we can calculate a measure of social “bridges” called brokerage. Degree and brokerage are just two examples of the many different things that we can calculate to study a person’s roles in a social network.

We can also use the information in a social network to calculate how close people in the network are to each other. This is an abstract type of distance. Suppose that we pick two random students, Felicity and Marco, in a school’s social network. We can calculate the distance between Felicity and Marco using the information about who their friends are. If Felicity and Marco are friends with each other, there is a distance of 1 between them. If they are not friends with each other, but Felicity is friends with Amy who is friends with Marco, then Marco and Felicity are “friends of friends,” so there is a distance of 2 between them (see Figure 1).

Studying Brains

How can we relate the rich information in a social network to how the people in that network use their brains? One way that scientists have begun to study this question is by collecting and examining brain data from people in the same social network. Scientists often collect brain data using a technique called functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). A magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner uses powerful magnets to take pictures that capture a person’s brain activity while they are doing a task that a researcher has asked them to do. Examples of such tasks are watching a movie, viewing pictures of people, or playing a game. For more information about how an MRI scanner works, see [3].

By combining fMRI data with information about people’s social networks, scientists can study many interesting questions (see Figure 2). Try thinking about a specific student at your school. What types of information come to your mind? You might think about what this person looks like or if they are funny or kind. You might also think about what role this person plays in your social world. Is this person popular? Is this person good at connecting people from different friendship groups? Such features characterize the person’s social-network position. Scientists have shown that our brains automatically process information about other people’s social-network positions when we see people who we know (see Figure 3) [1]. Why would our brains automatically do this? One reason may be that keeping track of other people’s social-network positions can help us navigate our social worlds. For example, perhaps you only want to share a secret with your closest friends. Additionally, it could be risky to tell a secret to the most popular student at school because this person is connected to a lot of other people.

Figure 2 - How scientists study social networks and brains together.
  • Figure 2 - How scientists study social networks and brains together.
  • Scientists often use surveys to collect information about people’s social networks. For example, scientists can ask people to list their friends, and they can then use the responses to their questions to characterize those people’s social networks. Scientists can also obtain information about how people’s brains respond to the world by measuring their brain activity using fMRI. Scientists can then combine the information from brain scans with information about people’s social networks to study how our brains and social worlds interact with each other. (Image credit: Flaticon.com)
Figure 3 - People keep track of social connections.
  • Figure 3 - People keep track of social connections.
  • When you see a friend, your brain automatically keeps track of their social-network characteristics. For example, Felicity’s brain calculates Marco’s degree. Because Marco is connected to two people, Marco’s degree is 2. When Felicity sees Marco, her brain calculates the distance between them in their social network. Marco is a friend of a friend of Felicity, so the distance between them is 2. (Image credit: Adobe Stock File #221784609)

Scientists have discovered that people in certain social-network positions use their brains differently in specific situations. For example, recent research has revealed that the brains of people with a large degree (that is, popular people) track the degrees of other people especially closely [4]. In other words, popular people may pay particularly close attention to the popularity of their peers. Scientists have also studied whether or not people with large brokerage use their brains differently. In one study, researchers found that the brains of people with large brokerage were particularly sensitive to determining when other people’s opinions were different from theirs [4]. These people had especially elevated activity in brain areas that are involved in thinking about what other people are thinking about. This is a very important function for people with large brokerage because they provide social bridges to connect groups of people who may have different beliefs or opinions.

Scientists have also compared how the brains of people in the same social network process the world around them. In one study, researchers examined a social network of business-school students [5]. Some of these students participated in an fMRI investigation in which they watched a bunch of movie clips. Students who were closer together in the social network (for example, students who were friends) had more similar brain activity, and students who were farther apart had less similar brain activity. Friends had especially similar brain activity in brain regions that are involved in processing emotions and interpreting movies. This study suggests that people who are closer to each other in their social network may think in similar ways when they watch movies and perhaps also when they experience other things. Consider the similarities between you and your friends. Do you and your friends laugh at the same jokes or feel similar emotions when you watch movies? This may reflect similarities in how you and your friends think about the world around you.

Even though scientists have found that people in different social-network positions have brains that operate differently, they have not yet discovered if differences in the brain lead to people occupying particular social-network positions or if occupying particular social-network positions causes changes in the brain [1]. This is hard to resolve in humans. For instance, it would be very difficult to randomly assign many friends to one person and no friends to some other person. Even if it were possible to randomly assign people to different social-network positions, it would be unethical to do so. For example, forcing an individual to have a smaller number of friends may cause that person to feel distressed. (For information about the role of ethics in studying brains, see [6].) Scientists have also studied other animals (such as macaque monkeys) to gain insights into the relationship between brain differences and social-network positions. One group of researchers studied macaques who had been placed in social networks of different sizes and used an MRI scanner to measure the monkeys’ brain structure and activity after about 1 year [7]. They found that monkeys in larger social networks developed differences in the structure and activity of brain regions that help them (and also help humans) make sense of what others are thinking. Although it is not yet known if such relationships work the same way in humans, this study demonstrates the potential power of social networks to influence the brain.

Conclusions

Scientists simultaneously study the brain and social networks to investigate how our brains influence and are influenced by the complex social world in which we live. Such research has helped us learn how our brains support our ability to keep track of people in our social networks and monitor other people’s relationships with each other. Such research can also help scientists understand how and why people in different social-network positions may process the world around them in different ways. Combining the study of brains and social networks is a new area of research, and interesting new studies in this area are coming out all of the time. What questions would you be interested in answering if you could measure people’s brain activity and study their social networks?

Glossary

Social Network: A network in which, typically, each node represents a person and each edge represents some type of connection (such as a friendship) between two people.

Node: An object in a network. It is connected to other objects through edges. An example is a person in a social network.

Edge: A connection between two nodes in a network. An example is a friendship between two people in a social network.

Degree: The number of connections of a node in a network. For example, the degree of a person indicates how many friends they have in a social network.

Brokerage: A measure of how well a person in a social network connects (“bridges”) different groups of people in the network.

Distance: How far apart two nodes are from each other in a network. (This is an abstract type of distance.)

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): A way of measuring what is going on in a person’s brain as the person performs a task. To learn how an MRI scanner works, see [3].

Social-Network Position: The set of roles that an individual plays in a social network. These roles can relate to various things, including who their friends are, whether or not they have many friends, or whether or not they “bridge” people who otherwise might not be connected to each other.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to our young readers—Nia Chiou, Taryn Chiou, Zoe Chiou, Leia Chung, Valerie K. Eng, Maple Leung, and Meredith Ooi—for their many helpful comments. We also thank their parents, teachers, and friends (especially Lyndie Chiou and Christina Chow) for putting us in touch with them and soliciting their feedback. We are also grateful to our paper’s young reviewers and their mentors for their many helpful comments. We acknowledge support from the National Science Foundation SBE Postdoctoral Research Fellowship [Grant No. 1911783] and the National Science Foundation [Grant No. SBE-1835239 and Grant No. SBE-2048212].


References

[1] Baek, E. C., Porter, M. A., and Parkinson, C. 2021. Social network analysis for social neuroscientists. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 16:883–901. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsaa069

[2] Holme, P., Porter, M. A., and Sayama, H. 2019. Who is the most important character in Frozen? What networks can tell us about the world. Front. Young Minds 7:99. doi: 10.3389/frym.2019.00099

[3] Hoyos, P., Kim, N., and Kastner, S. 2019. How is magnetic resonance imaging used to learn about the brain? Front. Young Minds 7:86. doi: 10.3389/frym.2019.00086

[4] O’Donnell, M. B., Bayer, J. B., Cascio, C. N., and Falk, E. B. 2017. Neural bases of recommendations differ according to social network structure. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12:61–9. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw158

[5] Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M., and Wheatley, T. 2018. Similar neural responses predict friendship. Nat. Commun. 9:332. doi: 10.1038/s41467-017-02722-7

[6] Mc Glanaghy, E., Di Pietro, N., and Illes, J. 2015. The brain and ethics: An introduction to research in neuroethics. Front. Young Minds 3:2. doi: 10.3389/frym.2015.00002

[7] Sallet, J., Mars, R. B., Noonan, M. P., Noonan, M. P., O’Reilly, J. X., Jbabdi, S., et al. 2011. Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques. Science 334:697–700. doi: 10.1126/science.1210027