Do you enjoy building airplanes, cars, houses, or robots with Lego blocks? Humans are the only animal species that can create complicated constructions from simple Lego blocks – our Lego building ability is “human-specific,” since it is only found in human beings. What would our closest relatives, apes or monkeys, do with a box of Lego blocks? They would probably chew on them, and lose interest when they find out that they are not edible! Why are humans the only Lego builders in the animal kingdom? What happens in our brains when we build a Lego construction?
Let us first take a look at how humans differ from other animals.
How are Humans Different from Other Animals?
Humans can do a number of things that no other animals – not even our closest relatives (such as chimps and gorillas) – can do. We are the only species that has developed languages with a set of rules (a grammar) that requires words to be in a certain order. You might have seen monkeys calling each other (for example, a “koo” call is signaling friendliness), but you have never seen one writing a letter and wondering about spelling! We are also able to predict from the look of a friend's face or the sound of his/her voice how he/she feels about the world, whether he/she is happy or sad. In addition, we pass from generation to generation the knowledge that we have learned about the world and our universe – this is why we go to school! Going to school and teaching children about the world is part of the human “culture,” another human-specific characteristic. Language, predicting a friend's mood, and culture are all examples of “human-specific” abilities. Why can we do all these things and other animals cannot? It is because our brain has developed extra machinery for these abilities: there are specialized parts of the brain that produce language or predict a friend's mood (see Figure 1).
In this article, we want to describe another ability that we think is human-specific: to build, use, and know about tools. But first, are we really the only tool users in the animal kingdom?
Humans are Not the Only Tool Users but the Most Intelligent Ones!
Many kinds of animals use objects that they find in their environment for a specific purpose. Thus, they are using the objects as “tools” [1] (see Figure 2). For example, the Egyptian vulture picks up rocks with its beak and uses them to pound the shell of an ostrich egg. When it cracks, the vulture can eat its favorite food. Octopuses carry around shells (sometimes even coconut shells) to hide underneath, or they tear off tentacles from jellyfish and wield them as a weapon when attacked. Chimps, our closest relatives, strip leaves off twigs to use as sticks for fishing termites out of a termite mound. The examples are endless. However, no chimp can do what a 2-year old child can do: use the stick for a different purpose if the situation asks for it. For example, a child might use a drumstick that is lying around to get a ball that rolled underneath the sofa. The child can do this because he/she is able to plan ahead and understand what the stick will do to the ball. So even though humans are not the only tool users, we are the only ones able to use tools in a highly intelligent way!
Not only are we very creative in transforming simple objects like Lego into complicated constructions like airplanes and robots but we are also very smart about making complicated tools for very specific purposes. We even build tools that work together in specific ways – like screws and screwdrivers or hammers and nails – and we design machines that can make these tools. We can even program computers that run the machines, which gives us free time to build even more!
But how did it all start?
The Origins of Tool Technology
Our ability to use tools in an intelligent way can even be seen in the earliest human tools we have found, which are made of stone. We think that early hominid (human-like) species, which were our extinct ancestors, used tools made from organic materials like sticks, leaves, and wood before using stone, but we cannot find any traces of them because these materials decay. This is why the first objects we know were used as tools are stones [2]. The oldest known stone tools were found in Ethiopia (Africa) about 2.5 million years ago. Typical stone tools are shown in Figures 3A,B. The stone tools that have been found clearly show that they have been made by intelligent toolmakers. Hominids made these tools by hitting one rock (called “core stone”) with a “hammer stone,” thereby knocking off flakes (see Figure 3A). Both the flakes and the cores of the stones were used as tools. The flakes had sharp edges, so they could be used as cutting tools.
In order to break flakes from the core, the core stone had to be hit by the hammer stone just at the right angle. This means that the core and hammer stones had to be held firmly and that force had to be applied in a very precise way. Humans and their ancestors could only accomplish this because they have what we call a precision grip, which means that our thumb can touch the tips of our other fingers. The way that stone tools were prepared shows that our ancestors understood the characteristics of the stones and how they could use the resulting tools on other objects (like using a sharp-edged stone to slice meat). Importantly, there is evidence that the early hominids taught each other how to produce stone tools, which they passed to the next generation – they developed a culture of using and building tools.
Stone tool technology did not only change the different grips that our hands can accomplish, but it also changed our brain (see Figure 4).
Our Brain has Specialized Machinery for Knowing and Using Tools (Call it Our “Lego Building Network”)
What happens in your brain when you use tools, or build a complicated Lego construction? Different parts of your brain are specialized in processing information from different senses (see Figure 5). When you look at Lego blocks, what you see is sent from your eyes to the back of your brain, where the visual cortex is located (in the occipital cortex, see Figure 5A). The visual cortex will extract things like shape, size, and color of the blocks. Once you have decided what you want to build, the motor cortex that controls all movements (located in the frontal cortex, see Figure 5) will tell your muscles what to do and in what order. But the motor cortex needs to know where the Lego blocks are (an arm length away and piled up, the one you want to use first is in the middle) and how they are oriented (the long end is facing you). The parietal cortex, which is located between the visual cortex and the motor cortex (see Figure 5), translates sight information into something that the motor cortex can understand. This is how you will be able to stretch your arm the right length and grasp the Lego block from the correct side.
Our brain and the brains of monkeys and apes are pretty similar when it comes to recognizing and handling objects. However, only in our brain, there is additional machinery that is specialized in processing information about objects that we use as tools. Think of it as a “Lego building network,” or a special computer program. It involves several regions (“tool regions”) all over the brain that communicate with each other and form a network (see Figure 5B). Scientists believe that this special machinery allows us to use tools in a highly intelligent way. How exactly each of the tool regions contributes to our tool use abilities is part of ongoing research (see Box 1 for an example of how scientists identify and study tool regions in the brain).
So Why are We the Only Lego Builders in the Animal Kingdom?
Certain areas in our brains (along with other parts of our body, like our hands) have developed further than in other animals’ brains, which allows us to do things that no other animal can do. Comparing human and other animals’ brains helps scientists find out what makes our brains special. The better we understand how our brain works, the better we can find solutions to help people who have difficulties in doing certain things.
References
[1] ↑ Shumaker, R. W., Walkup, K. R., and Beck, B. B. 2011. Animal Tool Behavior: The Use and Manufacture of Tools by Animals. Baltimore, MD: The John Hopkins University Press.
[2] ↑ Zimmer, C. 2005. Smithsonian Intimate Guide to Human Origins. Toronto: Madison Press Books.
[3] ↑ Mruczek, R. E. B., von Loga, I. S., Shariat Torbaghan, S., and Kastner, S. 2013. The representation of tool and non-tool object information in the human intraparietal sulcus. J. Neurophysiol. 109:2883–2896. doi: 10.1152/jn.00658.2012